Open Access Policy Deposits
Parent: Department of Linguistics
eScholarship stats: History by Item for December, 2024 through March, 2025
Item | Title | Total requests | 2025-03 | 2025-02 | 2025-01 | 2024-12 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
05b2s4wg | The empirical base of linguistics: Grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology | 165 | 49 | 48 | 38 | 30 |
7f26m713 | Challenges for a theory of islands: A broader perspective on Ambridge, Pine, and Lieven | 142 | 15 | 48 | 34 | 45 |
04r5q6qn | WPP, No. 105: Linguistic Voice Quality | 90 | 16 | 27 | 21 | 26 |
9xx930j1 | WPP, No. 108: Phonation Contrasts Across Languages | 71 | 9 | 16 | 21 | 25 |
9pn2b9qm | Measures of the glottal source spectrum. | 66 | 14 | 23 | 14 | 15 |
0n76t3hn | WPP, No.111: Glottal articulations of phonation contrasts and their acoustic and perceptual consequences | 64 | 13 | 20 | 17 | 14 |
1xn120qk | When is a Verb not a Verb? | 55 | 12 | 21 | 10 | 12 |
8dr06327 | Perception of aperiodicity in pathological voice | 50 | 13 | 13 | 12 | 12 |
29s5h9w3 | Information conveyed by voice qualitya) | 47 | 8 | 17 | 10 | 12 |
4mt0s60w | Measuring vocal quality with speech synthesis. | 46 | 11 | 18 | 9 | 8 |
90d6532f | WPP, No. 104: Intonational Phonology of Seoul Korean Revisited | 46 | 11 | 10 | 10 | 15 |
92d624qq | “Narten formations” versus “Narten roots” | 46 | 9 | 20 | 11 | 6 |
60v8v517 | Dependent-case assignment could be AGREE | 45 | 13 | 15 | 8 | 9 |
88x529wr | A comparison of informal and formal acceptability judgments using a random sample from Linguistic Inquiry 2001–2010 | 45 | 12 | 11 | 14 | 8 |
1gs6h5k7 | WPP, No. 108: The acoustic consequences of phonation and tone interactions in Jalapa Mazatec | 41 | 12 | 12 | 6 | 11 |
52g5f2dq | More on (the Lack of) Reconstruction in English Tough-Constructions | 41 | 12 | 16 | 6 | 7 |
7x39t5hg | Three levels of the symbolosphere | 40 | 13 | 12 | 3 | 12 |
61f416jp | fMRI reveals language-specific predictive coding during naturalistic sentence comprehension | 39 | 13 | 11 | 6 | 9 |
0xd5n4gk | High-toned [il] in Korean: Phonetics, intonational phonology, and sound change | 38 | 4 | 11 | 14 | 9 |
2mw9c40q | Non-literal language processing is jointly supported by the language and theory of mind networks: Evidence from a novel meta-analytic fMRI approach. | 37 | 17 | 12 | 4 | 4 |
3fq7x9mb | Vocal fold vibratory patterns in tense versus lax phonation contrasts | 37 | 7 | 18 | 10 | 2 |
4qs31528 | WPP, No. 108: Perception of pitch location within a speaker’s own range: fundamental frequency, voice quality and speaker sex | 37 | 7 | 12 | 10 | 8 |
5dp6b6s4 | Not all reconstruction effects are syntactic | 37 | 15 | 13 | 7 | 2 |
9w85x4mc | Validity of rating scale measures of voice quality. | 37 | 7 | 14 | 7 | 9 |
0n100842 | Grammar and the use of data | 36 | 10 | 14 | 10 | 2 |
46v4j8dt | Probabilistic atlas for the language network based on precision fMRI data from >800 individuals | 36 | 21 | 4 | 9 | 2 |
9n26b70j | Information Structure Preferences in Focus-Sensitive Ellipsis: How Defaults Persist | 36 | 9 | 12 | 10 | 5 |
3987s5jj | Differential Tracking of Linguistic vs. Mental State Content in Naturalistic Stimuli by Language and Theory of Mind (ToM) Brain Networks. | 34 | 15 | 11 | 5 | 3 |
5tj8f292 | Against some approaches to long-distance agreement without AGREE | 34 | 6 | 10 | 10 | 8 |
9mt1x9z7 | A meta-analytic review of morphological priming in Semitic languages | 34 | 9 | 7 | 9 | 9 |
8k4092h0 | Sources of listener disagreement in voice quality assessment. | 33 | 5 | 14 | 7 | 7 |
0hc673ms | Synchronic and diachronic microvariation in English do | 31 | 8 | 9 | 7 | 7 |
69r4g5xd | Parameterizing passive participle movement | 31 | 4 | 14 | 7 | 6 |
8r206459 | Toward a unified theory of voice production and perception | 31 | 4 | 9 | 8 | 10 |
0h9362zf | Validating a psychoacoustic model of voice quality. | 30 | 10 | 10 | 4 | 6 |
75z0w33w | Why do children pay more attention to grammatical morphemes at the ends of sentences? | 30 | 4 | 10 | 4 | 12 |
7h89m6pn | Eighteen-month-old infants represent nonlocal syntactic dependencies | 30 | 7 | 8 | 7 | 8 |
0r69t4tr | WPP, No. 103: Linguistic Phonetics in the UCLA Phonetics Lab | 29 | 11 | 9 | 4 | 5 |
4kz9d7v0 | No evidence for differences among language regions in their temporal receptive windows | 29 | 6 | 12 | 7 | 4 |
50d070bw | Degree modification in natural language | 27 | 6 | 6 | 5 | 10 |
6mt3d01x | Semantic projection recovers rich human knowledge of multiple object features from word embeddings. | 27 | 10 | 12 | 5 | |
82m3995f | The perceptual structure of pathologic voice quality. | 27 | 3 | 10 | 6 | 8 |
5bc7k7xd | WPP, No.110: Perception of spectral slopes and tone identification in White Hmong | 26 | 6 | 6 | 8 | 6 |
6tg5z1jk | The polysemy of measurement | 26 | 3 | 14 | 2 | 7 |
98j5s1r7 | Keep it local (and final): Remnant preferences in “let alone” ellipsis | 26 | 5 | 11 | 6 | 4 |
1640t82j | Vocal Fundamental Frequency and Sound Pressure Level in Charismatic Speech: A Cross-Gender and -Language Study. | 25 | 6 | 10 | 6 | 3 |
3hq140vh | Acoustic voice variation within and between speakers. | 25 | 10 | 6 | 5 | 4 |
8rv9t589 | WPP, No. 106: Effects of Initial Position versus Prominence in English | 25 | 7 | 7 | 8 | 3 |
1gh6x943 | WPP, No. 108: Comparison of speaking fundamental frequency in English and Mandarin | 24 | 2 | 10 | 6 | 6 |
8b12461j | WPP, No. 103: Speech Perception in Dyslexic Children With and Without Language Impairments | 24 | 7 | 3 | 4 | 10 |
Note: Due to the evolving nature of web traffic, the data presented here should be considered approximate and subject to revision. Learn more.