Open Access Policy Deposits
Parent: Department of Linguistics
eScholarship stats: History by Item for September through December, 2024
Item | Title | Total requests | 2024-12 | 2024-11 | 2024-10 | 2024-09 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
7f26m713 | Challenges for a theory of islands: A broader perspective on Ambridge, Pine, and Lieven | 99 | 45 | 14 | 21 | 19 |
04r5q6qn | WPP, No. 105: Linguistic Voice Quality | 82 | 26 | 23 | 25 | 8 |
9pn2b9qm | Measures of the glottal source spectrum. | 61 | 15 | 16 | 19 | 11 |
9xx930j1 | WPP, No. 108: Phonation Contrasts Across Languages | 54 | 25 | 7 | 13 | 9 |
05b2s4wg | The empirical base of linguistics: Grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology | 52 | 30 | 6 | 12 | 4 |
0n76t3hn | WPP, No.111: Glottal articulations of phonation contrasts and their acoustic and perceptual consequences | 51 | 14 | 7 | 20 | 10 |
8vk1j9n6 | WPP, No. 106: Intonational Phonology of Georgian | 51 | 5 | 1 | 42 | 3 |
1xn120qk | When is a Verb not a Verb? | 44 | 12 | 11 | 13 | 8 |
90d6532f | WPP, No. 104: Intonational Phonology of Seoul Korean Revisited | 44 | 15 | 13 | 7 | 9 |
0xd5n4gk | High-toned [il] in Korean: Phonetics, intonational phonology, and sound change | 39 | 9 | 12 | 8 | 10 |
4qs31528 | WPP, No. 108: Perception of pitch location within a speaker’s own range: fundamental frequency, voice quality and speaker sex | 36 | 8 | 8 | 16 | 4 |
60v8v517 | Dependent-case assignment could be AGREE | 36 | 9 | 7 | 11 | 9 |
92d624qq | “Narten formations” versus “Narten roots” | 36 | 6 | 9 | 17 | 4 |
29s5h9w3 | Information conveyed by voice qualitya) | 31 | 12 | 8 | 7 | 4 |
50d070bw | Degree modification in natural language | 31 | 10 | 7 | 11 | 3 |
82m3995f | The perceptual structure of pathologic voice quality. | 31 | 8 | 3 | 14 | 6 |
0hc673ms | Synchronic and diachronic microvariation in English do | 30 | 7 | 8 | 8 | 7 |
75z0w33w | Why do children pay more attention to grammatical morphemes at the ends of sentences? | 30 | 12 | 11 | 5 | 2 |
7x39t5hg | Three levels of the symbolosphere | 30 | 12 | 5 | 8 | 5 |
1gs6h5k7 | WPP, No. 108: The acoustic consequences of phonation and tone interactions in Jalapa Mazatec | 29 | 11 | 4 | 9 | 5 |
5tj8f292 | Against some approaches to long-distance agreement without AGREE | 29 | 8 | 6 | 5 | 10 |
85z926p7 | WPP, No. 104: Prosody in Sentence Processing: Korean vs. English | 28 | 10 | 7 | 8 | 3 |
88x529wr | A comparison of informal and formal acceptability judgments using a random sample from Linguistic Inquiry 2001–2010 | 28 | 8 | 7 | 9 | 4 |
1dq08371 | Subject Case in Children With SLI and Unaffected Controls: Evidence for the Agr/Tns Omission Model | 27 | 2 | 10 | 9 | 6 |
1gh6x943 | WPP, No. 108: Comparison of speaking fundamental frequency in English and Mandarin | 27 | 6 | 5 | 9 | 7 |
6tg5z1jk | The polysemy of measurement | 27 | 7 | 1 | 12 | 7 |
8r206459 | Toward a unified theory of voice production and perception | 27 | 10 | 5 | 8 | 4 |
8dr06327 | Perception of aperiodicity in pathological voice | 26 | 12 | 2 | 6 | 6 |
9mt1x9z7 | A meta-analytic review of morphological priming in Semitic languages | 26 | 9 | 3 | 11 | 3 |
9w85x4mc | Validity of rating scale measures of voice quality. | 26 | 9 | 5 | 7 | 5 |
3xk61586 | Cue-shifting between acoustic cues: Evidence for directional asymmetry | 25 | 5 | 1 | 9 | 10 |
0h9362zf | Validating a psychoacoustic model of voice quality. | 24 | 6 | 7 | 6 | 5 |
4mt0s60w | Measuring vocal quality with speech synthesis. | 23 | 8 | 3 | 5 | 7 |
8k4092h0 | Sources of listener disagreement in voice quality assessment. | 23 | 7 | 2 | 11 | 3 |
61f416jp | fMRI reveals language-specific predictive coding during naturalistic sentence comprehension | 22 | 9 | 4 | 7 | 2 |
8b12461j | WPP, No. 103: Speech Perception in Dyslexic Children With and Without Language Impairments | 22 | 10 | 5 | 4 | 3 |
2bx789pq | A Multilab Study of Bilingual Infants: Exploring the Preference for Infant-Directed Speech | 21 | 4 | 4 | 9 | 4 |
74z3z4c3 | Stem similarity modulates infants' acquisition of phonological alternations | 21 | 6 | 5 | 7 | 3 |
1v3459mc | Measure Phrase Equatives and Modified Numerals | 20 | 8 | 4 | 8 | |
4kz9d7v0 | No evidence for differences among language regions in their temporal receptive windows | 19 | 4 | 2 | 8 | 5 |
8hb954bq | Serbo-Croatian clitic placement: An argument for prosodic movement | 19 | 2 | 6 | 7 | 4 |
0n100842 | Grammar and the use of data | 18 | 2 | 5 | 6 | 5 |
0r69t4tr | WPP, No. 103: Linguistic Phonetics in the UCLA Phonetics Lab | 18 | 5 | 5 | 4 | 4 |
3fq7x9mb | Vocal fold vibratory patterns in tense versus lax phonation contrasts | 18 | 2 | 2 | 7 | 7 |
52g5f2dq | More on (the Lack of) Reconstruction in English Tough-Constructions | 18 | 7 | 8 | 3 | |
5bc7k7xd | WPP, No.110: Perception of spectral slopes and tone identification in White Hmong | 18 | 6 | 2 | 8 | 2 |
65v911tq | Improper case | 18 | 4 | 6 | 8 | |
6608d4ft | Intervention in tough-constructions revisited | 18 | 4 | 3 | 7 | 4 |
7h89m6pn | Eighteen-month-old infants represent nonlocal syntactic dependencies. | 18 | 8 | 4 | 5 | 1 |
1qm548f8 | Exposure to a second language in infancy alters speech production | 17 | 9 | 5 | 2 | 1 |
Note: Due to the evolving nature of web traffic, the data presented here should be considered approximate and subject to revision. Learn more.