Open Access Policy Deposits
Parent: Department of Linguistics
eScholarship stats: Breakdown by Item for September through December, 2024
Item | Title | Total requests | Download | View-only | %Dnld |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
7f26m713 | Challenges for a theory of islands: A broader perspective on Ambridge, Pine, and Lieven | 99 | 3 | 96 | 3.0% |
04r5q6qn | WPP, No. 105: Linguistic Voice Quality | 82 | 3 | 79 | 3.7% |
9pn2b9qm | Measures of the glottal source spectrum. | 61 | 14 | 47 | 23.0% |
9xx930j1 | WPP, No. 108: Phonation Contrasts Across Languages | 54 | 12 | 42 | 22.2% |
05b2s4wg | The empirical base of linguistics: Grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology | 52 | 35 | 17 | 67.3% |
0n76t3hn | WPP, No.111: Glottal articulations of phonation contrasts and their acoustic and perceptual consequences | 51 | 12 | 39 | 23.5% |
8vk1j9n6 | WPP, No. 106: Intonational Phonology of Georgian | 51 | 43 | 8 | 84.3% |
1xn120qk | When is a Verb not a Verb? | 44 | 12 | 32 | 27.3% |
90d6532f | WPP, No. 104: Intonational Phonology of Seoul Korean Revisited | 44 | 6 | 38 | 13.6% |
0xd5n4gk | High-toned [il] in Korean: Phonetics, intonational phonology, and sound change | 39 | 11 | 28 | 28.2% |
4qs31528 | WPP, No. 108: Perception of pitch location within a speaker’s own range: fundamental frequency, voice quality and speaker sex | 36 | 5 | 31 | 13.9% |
60v8v517 | Dependent-case assignment could be AGREE | 36 | 3 | 33 | 8.3% |
92d624qq | “Narten formations” versus “Narten roots” | 36 | 2 | 34 | 5.6% |
29s5h9w3 | Information conveyed by voice qualitya) | 31 | 19 | 12 | 61.3% |
50d070bw | Degree modification in natural language | 31 | 22 | 9 | 71.0% |
82m3995f | The perceptual structure of pathologic voice quality. | 31 | 17 | 14 | 54.8% |
0hc673ms | Synchronic and diachronic microvariation in English do | 30 | 6 | 24 | 20.0% |
75z0w33w | Why do children pay more attention to grammatical morphemes at the ends of sentences? | 30 | 25 | 5 | 83.3% |
7x39t5hg | Three levels of the symbolosphere | 30 | 22 | 8 | 73.3% |
1gs6h5k7 | WPP, No. 108: The acoustic consequences of phonation and tone interactions in Jalapa Mazatec | 29 | 5 | 24 | 17.2% |
5tj8f292 | Against some approaches to long-distance agreement without AGREE | 29 | 9 | 20 | 31.0% |
85z926p7 | WPP, No. 104: Prosody in Sentence Processing: Korean vs. English | 28 | 6 | 22 | 21.4% |
88x529wr | A comparison of informal and formal acceptability judgments using a random sample from Linguistic Inquiry 2001–2010 | 28 | 21 | 7 | 75.0% |
1dq08371 | Subject Case in Children With SLI and Unaffected Controls: Evidence for the Agr/Tns Omission Model | 27 | 19 | 8 | 70.4% |
1gh6x943 | WPP, No. 108: Comparison of speaking fundamental frequency in English and Mandarin | 27 | 6 | 21 | 22.2% |
6tg5z1jk | The polysemy of measurement | 27 | 17 | 10 | 63.0% |
8r206459 | Toward a unified theory of voice production and perception | 27 | 3 | 24 | 11.1% |
8dr06327 | Perception of aperiodicity in pathological voice | 26 | 16 | 10 | 61.5% |
9mt1x9z7 | A meta-analytic review of morphological priming in Semitic languages | 26 | 15 | 11 | 57.7% |
9w85x4mc | Validity of rating scale measures of voice quality. | 26 | 10 | 16 | 38.5% |
3xk61586 | Cue-shifting between acoustic cues: Evidence for directional asymmetry | 25 | 9 | 16 | 36.0% |
0h9362zf | Validating a psychoacoustic model of voice quality. | 24 | 7 | 17 | 29.2% |
4mt0s60w | Measuring vocal quality with speech synthesis. | 23 | 11 | 12 | 47.8% |
8k4092h0 | Sources of listener disagreement in voice quality assessment. | 23 | 7 | 16 | 30.4% |
61f416jp | fMRI reveals language-specific predictive coding during naturalistic sentence comprehension | 22 | 4 | 18 | 18.2% |
8b12461j | WPP, No. 103: Speech Perception in Dyslexic Children With and Without Language Impairments | 22 | 4 | 18 | 18.2% |
2bx789pq | A Multilab Study of Bilingual Infants: Exploring the Preference for Infant-Directed Speech | 21 | 4 | 17 | 19.0% |
74z3z4c3 | Stem similarity modulates infants' acquisition of phonological alternations | 21 | 3 | 18 | 14.3% |
1v3459mc | Measure Phrase Equatives and Modified Numerals | 20 | 8 | 12 | 40.0% |
4kz9d7v0 | No evidence for differences among language regions in their temporal receptive windows | 19 | 5 | 14 | 26.3% |
8hb954bq | Serbo-Croatian clitic placement: An argument for prosodic movement | 19 | 6 | 13 | 31.6% |
0n100842 | Grammar and the use of data | 18 | 4 | 14 | 22.2% |
0r69t4tr | WPP, No. 103: Linguistic Phonetics in the UCLA Phonetics Lab | 18 | 4 | 14 | 22.2% |
3fq7x9mb | Vocal fold vibratory patterns in tense versus lax phonation contrasts | 18 | 9 | 9 | 50.0% |
52g5f2dq | More on (the Lack of) Reconstruction in English Tough-Constructions | 18 | 8 | 10 | 44.4% |
5bc7k7xd | WPP, No.110: Perception of spectral slopes and tone identification in White Hmong | 18 | 1 | 17 | 5.6% |
65v911tq | Improper case | 18 | 2 | 16 | 11.1% |
6608d4ft | Intervention in tough-constructions revisited | 18 | 4 | 14 | 22.2% |
7h89m6pn | Eighteen-month-old infants represent nonlocal syntactic dependencies | 18 | 1 | 17 | 5.6% |
1qm548f8 | Exposure to a second language in infancy alters speech production | 17 | 10 | 7 | 58.8% |
Note: Due to the evolving nature of web traffic, the data presented here should be considered approximate and subject to revision. Learn more.